Home Curriculum Vitae Publications Conference Reports Forthcoming Extramural Colloquia Expert Testimony Teaching Healthcare The Human Ecology of Memory Research Archive Publications and Reports Rants

 

Hypnosis in the Right Hemisphere

 

John F. Kihlstrom

University of California, Berkeley

Martha L. Glisky

Bellevue, Washington

Susan McGovern

Tucson, Arizona

Steven Z. Rapcsak

University of Arizona College of Medicine

Mark S. Mennemeier

University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences


Note: an edited version of this article was published in Cortex, 49, 393-399 (2013). 


Link to translation into Romanian by
Irina Vasilescu

Link to translation into Bulgarian by Sophia Yuzyna.

Link to translation into Russian by Sophia Yuzyna.

Link to translation into Swedish by Sophia Yuzyna.

Link to translation into Chinese (Mandarin) by Shirley Li


Abstract

 

Speculations about the neural substrates of hypnosis have often implicated the right hemisphere. If so, right-hemisphere damage should impair hypnotic responsiveness more than left-hemisphere damage. The present study examined the performance of a patient who suffered a stroke destroying most of his left hemisphere, on slightly modified versions of two hypnotizability scales. This patient was hypnotizable, as indicated in particular by the age regression and amnesia items, suggesting that hypnosis can be mediated by the right hemisphere alone -- provided that the language capacities normally found in the left hemisphere remain available. A further study of 16 patients with unilateral strokes of the left or right hemisphere found no substantial differences in hypnotizability between the two groups. Future neuropsychological studies of hypnosis might explore the dorsal/ventral or anterior/posterior dichotomies, with special emphasis on the role of prefrontal cortex.

 

Hypnosis in the Right Hemisphere

Of all the speculations concerning the neural substrates of hypnosis and hypnotizability, perhaps the most popular have been those that implicate the right cerebral hemisphere (for reviews, see Barabasz & Barabasz, 2008; Kihlstrom, 2011b). Based on an identification of hypnosis with the creative, intuitive, nonanalytic, and holistic processing often held to characterize right-hemisphere function (e.g., Hellige, 1993; but see Efron, 1990), in contrast to the logical, sequential, and analytic processing associated with the left hemisphere, Bakan (1969) proposed that hypnosis was mediated by the right hemisphere. Of course, a strong interpretation of the right-hemisphere hypothesis is untenable (Jasiukaitis, Nouriani, Hugdahl, & Spiegel, 1997), if for no other reason than that the comprehension of hypnotic suggestions requires linguistic skills normally associated with the left hemisphere. Nevertheless, over the succeeding years, the idea that the nondominant, typically right, hemisphere is somehow specialized for hypnosis has been extremely attractive to both researchers and theorists (for a comprehensive review, see H.J. Crawford & Gruzelier, 1992).

Evidence for right-hemisphere involvement in hypnosis has come mostly from studies employing behavioral or psychophysiological paradigms (for a comprehensive review, see Kihlstrom, 2011b). For example, Bakan himself reported that hypnotizable subjects showed more reflective eye movements to the left, ostensibly indicating greater right-hemisphere activation, than insusceptible subjects (Bakan, 1969), while other investigators found that hypnosis diminished the right-ear (i.e., left-hemisphere) advantage frequently found in dichotic listening tasks (Frumkin, Ripley, & Cox, 1978; Spellacy & Wilkinson, 1987). Hypnotizability has been associated with autokinetic movements to the left (Graham & Pernicano, 1979). In one study, subjects who sat on the right side of a room (thus placing the hypnotist in their left visual half-field, projecting into the right cerebral hemisphere) were more hypnotizable than those who sat on the left (Sackeim, Paulhus, & Weiman, 1979), while in another study right-handed subjects given motor suggestions were more responsive on the left side of their body --i.e., the side controlled by the right hemisphere (Sackeim, 1982). Hypnotizability has been correlated with performance on "gestalt closure" tasks hat seem to capitalize on the holistic information-processing capacities of the right hemisphere (Crawford, 1981); and the induction of hypnosis, particularly in hypnotizable subjects, enhanced performance on behavioral tasks that ostensibly capitalized on "right hemisphere" functions (Bakan, 1970; Crawford, 1986). On the other hand, many of these observations have proved difficult to confirm and extend (e.g., Bakan, 1970; Cranney & McConky, 1980; Gur & Gur, 1974; Monteiro & Zimbardo, 1987; Otto-Salaj, Nadon, Hoyt, Register, & Kihlstrom, 1992; Stam, Spanos, Radtke, & Jones, 1981; Wallace & Persanyi, 1989).

A similar fate befell psychophysiological and brain-imaging studies of hypnosis, hypnotizability, and laterality. Some investigators reported that hypnosis produced a shift from left- to right-hemisphere activation, as measured by the EEG (Edmonston & Moskovitz, 1990; MacLeod-Morgan & Lack, 1982), while Gruzelier and his colleagues found lateral asymmetries in EDR activity suggesting an inhibition of the left-hemisphere (Gruzelier, Brow, Perry, Rhonder, & Thomas, 1984; Gruzelier & Brow, 1985). At the same time, both earlier (Morgan, Macdonald, & Hilgard, 1974; Morgan, Macdonald, & Macdonald, 1971) and later (Graffin, Ray, & Lundy, 1995) studies failed to produce congruent results. A pioneering PET study by Crawford and her colleagues found that hypnotizable subjects showed dramatic increases in regional cerebral blood flow in the right hemisphere following hypnotic induction (Crawford, Gur, Skolnick, Gur, & Benson, 1993). However, more recent studies have indicated broader patterns of activation involving both cerebral hemispheres (Maquet et al., 1999; Rainville, Hofbauer, Paus, Bushnell, & Price, 1999).

In all this activity, it is somewhat remarkable that, to our knowledge, no investigator has ever addressed the right-hemisphere hypothesis by testing patients with lateralized brain injury. Some investigators have administered neuropsychological tests, including assessments of lateralized function, to hypnotized or hypnotizable subjects, (Gruzelier & Warren, 1993; Query, Carlson, & Dreyer, 1983), but these subjects were neurologically intact. Laidlaw (1993) assessed hypnotizability in a group of neurological patients who had suffered closed head injuries, but did not specifically consider laterality. Relatedly, Persinger and his colleagues (Healey, Persinger, & Koren, 1996; Tiller & Persinger, 1994) found that the brief application of a weak pulsed magnetic field over the right temporal lobe, inducing activity resembling complex partial epileptic-like seizures, increased hypnotizability in neurologically intact subjects. The present study sought to contribute to neuropsychological investigations of hypnosis by examining hypnotic susceptibility in patients with clearly lateralized brain damage secondary to stroke.

 

Case Study: Patient GK

At the time of testing (1994), GK was a 63-year-old right-handed male who had suffered a left-hemisphere stroke in 1977, resulting in global aphasia, alexia, and agraphia, as well as a dense right homonymous hemianopia, right hemiplegia, and right hemisensory loss. An MRI scan showed a massive infarction involving the distribution of all three major cerebral arteries supplying the left hemisphere. The lesion resulted in virtually complete destruction of the cortex of all four cerebral lobes of the left hemisphere as well as the underlying white matter. The right hemisphere appeared to be normal.

Following his discharge from the hospital, GK showed continuous improvement of his language and gestural abilities, reaching a plateau after about two years in recovery. At the time of testing, more than 15 years after his stroke, his speech and language abilities were consistent with Broca’s (expressive) aphasia. His spontaneous speech was effortful, non-fluent, and slightly dysarthric; his speech output was agrammatical but meaningful, with the omission of functors and simplified sentence structure. His auditory comprehension was excellent for conversational speech, disrupted only when he was dealing with complex sentences. GK walked with a leg brace, and his only other physical limitation was a right hemisensory loss, and a right hemiplegia restricting use of his right arm. Despite these impairments, he lived alone, tended to his daily affairs without any apparent difficulty, and pursued his hobby of building model automobiles and airplanes.

Because GK's language abilities were relatively well preserved, he was almost unique among those patients who have lost so much of their left hemisphere late in life. Accordingly, he was studied by investigators who were interested in hemispheric contributions to various aspects of cognitive and behavioral function (Polster & Rapcsak, 1994; Rapcsak, Beeson, & Rubens, 1991; Rapcsak, Ochipa, Beeson, & Rubens, 1993). Interested readers are referred to these publications for more details concerning his brain injury and profile of cognitive abilities.

In the course of events, GK agreed to participate in a study of hypnosis. For this purpose, he was individually administered modified versions of the 12-item Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale: Form A (SHSS:A; Weitzenhoffer & Hilgard, 1959), followed by the 12-item Stanford Form C (SHSS:C; Weitzenhoffer & Hilgard, 1962). In return for his participation, GK received an honorarium of $25 plus reimbursement of his travel expenses for each of two sessions lasting approximately 75 minutes in length.

In view of GK’s right hemiplegia, the Postural Sway item (#1) of SHSS:A was replaced by the corresponding "Head Falling Forward" item (#1) of the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form A (HGSHS:A; Shor & Orne, 1962, 1963). The Arm Immobilization item (#4) was shifted from the right to the left side, and the Hands Moving Together suggestion (#7) was omitted entirely. Two items, Posthypnotic Suggestion and Posthypnotic Amnesia, proved difficult to assess due to GK's physical and verbal-expressive limitations. Still, GK clearly passed nine out of the possible 11 items, which would ordinarily indicate relatively high hypnotizability (Register & Kihlstrom, 1986).

In contrast to his relatively high score on SHSS:A, however, GK clearly passed only four out of a possible 10 items on a modification of the more demanding SHSS:C. As on SHSS:A, lateralized items of the SHSS:C (#1, Hand Lowering; #5, Arm Rigidity; and #8, Arm Immobilization) were shifted as appropriate; Hands Moving Apart (#2) was eliminated. In view of GK's expressive aphasia, the Dream (#6) item was also eliminated, leaving 10 items in the scale. For the same reason, a recognition test was added to the usual recall test of posthypnotic amnesia (Kihlstrom & Shor, 1978).

Five items were common to both the Form A and the Form C. GK passed two of these on both occasions: the relatively easy Hand Lowering suggestion and the more difficult Arm Rigidity. He barely failed Arm Immobilization on SHSS:C, lifting his hand about three inches before the end of the 10-second interval. On the Mosquito Hallucination item of SHSS:C (corresponding to the Fly Hallucination of SHSS:A), he appeared to be annoyed but did not make any swatting gesture. Perhaps this was due to his physical limitations, but in any event he was scored as failing the suggestion. On the test of posthypnotic amnesia, GK recalled nothing while the amnesia suggestion was in effect, but on the recognition test correctly identified seven suggestions, with no false positives; accordingly, he was scored as failing the amnesia item.

Interestingly, GK responded positively to the cognitively demanding Age Regression suggestion on SHSS:C. This was illustrated both by his verbal report of his name and age (nine), and by his handwriting sample. He wrote the year as 1939 (which was when he was nine years old), and named and described his teacher.

GK's response in both hypnosis sessions indicated that he was at least somewhat hypnotizable. Although he did not pass a majority of the items on SHSS:C, which is generally considered the "gold standard" for the assessment of hypnotizability (Register & Kihlstrom, 1986), he did score relatively high on SHSS:A and responded positively to two of the more difficult suggestions -- Arm Rigidity and Age Regression -- on SHSS:C.

 

An Exploratory Study of Stroke Patients

Nothing about GK's performance would suggest that the special abilities associated with the right hemisphere offer any particular advantage, so far as hypnosis is concerned. At the same time, however special, GK was only one subject. Accordingly, we followed our study of GK with a pilot study of patients who had suffered unilateral brain damage as a result of a stroke.

 

Patients

The subjects in this study were 16 right-handed patients, 8 men and 8 women, averaging 56 years of age (SD = 16.40), members of a stroke survivor support group at a large university medical center who volunteered for a study of hypnosis. All signed a written informed consent to participate that was approved by the local institutional review board for the use of human subjects in research. All subjects were in the chronic stage of stroke: a minimum of six months had passed since the onset of their lesion.

Nine of these subjects (5 men, 4 women) had damage due to strokes affecting the left cerebral hemisphere (LH), and seven (3 men, 4 women) had damage due to strokes affecting the right hemisphere (RH). All exhibited behavioral evidence of unilateral stroke, including contralateral hemiparesis or sensory impairment and/or speech and language dysfunction characteristic of LH stroke. None of the subjects exhibited frank unilateral spatial neglect or anosognosia and none had comprehension deficits that precluded understanding instructions. Clinically obtained neuroradiological images were available for the majority of subjects: they confirmed the presence of a unilateral stroke in 7/9 subjects with LH injury and in 4/7 subjects with RH injury. Table 1 shows the distribution of the lesions in the two groups. Most had lesions in the frontal, temporal, or parietal regions.

<<<<<Table 1 About Here>>>>>

The patients' average score on the Mini-Mental Status Exam (MMSE; Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975) was 25.75 (SD = 3.97), indicating essentially intact cognitive abilities. There was no difference in MMSE performance between the LH and RH groups (t < 1). In return for their participation, the patients received an honorarium of $25 plus reimbursement of their travel expenses for a single experimental session lasting 75 minutes.

 

Method

Following informed consent, each patient received an individual administration of the Arizona Motor Scale of Hypnotizability (AMSH), which consists of an induction of hypnosis accompanied by suggestions for 16 representative hypnotic experiences. The AMSH itself was derived from existing standardized tests of hypnotizability, such as SHSS:A and C and HGSHS:A: it is so named because it focuses on ideomotor suggestions of two types: direct and challenge. Each of the 16 test suggestions (including one for eye closure administered during the hypnotic induction procedure itself) is scored dichotomously (pass/fail) on the basis of objective behavioral criteria, yielding scores that can range from 0 to 16 points. Normative information based on 100 college-student subjects, and the AMSH script, is available as an internet resource (Kihlstrom, 2011a).

Many of the AMSH suggestions are lateralized, targeting either the left or the right arm or hand: as with GK, these suggestions were modified to take account of the patients' hemiplegia. As before, three suggestions involving bilateral movements were eliminated entirely: for purposes of comparison; scores on these items, and occasional missing data, were estimated by regression based on the normative sample.

 

Results

AMSH was first scored according to the standard criteria established in the published scales from which it was derived (Table 2). Despite differences in age, not to mention neurological status, the average AMSH score for the stroke patients was only slightly lower than that of the normative group of college students (t < 1).

<<<<<Table 2 About Here>>>>>

It is conventional to classify hypnotic suggestions into two major categories: "Ideomotor" suggestions, as their name implies, involve suggestions for bodily movements (e.g., the subject's head is falling forward); "cognitive" suggestions focus on changes in perception and memory (e.g., for age regression) -- which, of course, may also have consequences for behavior (Kihlstrom, 2008). Ideomotor suggestions, in turn, come in two major types: "direct" suggestions facilitate motor activity, as when it is suggested that the subject's outstretched hand is growing heavy and falling; "challenge" suggestions inhibit motor activity, as when it is suggested that the subject's arm is rigid, and he cannot bend it. Excluding the direct suggestion for eye closure, which is administered during the hypnotic induction procedure itself, AMSH contains six direct suggestions, six challenge suggestions, and three cognitive suggestions. There were no significant differences between the patients and the students on any of these subscales: Direct, t(114) = 1.01, n.s.; Challenge and Cognitive, both t < 1.

Table 2 also shows that the two groups of stroke patients did not differ significantly from each other, in terms of total scale score (t < 1), or any of the three types of items: Direct, t(14) = 1.81, p < .10; Challenge, t < 1.; Cognitive, t < 1. Contrary to the laterality hypothesis, there was a tendency for subjects with RH lesions to score higher than those with LH lesions; however, the difference was not statistically significant (t < 1).

Compared to direct suggestions, challenge suggestions are more complex, with a three-step structure: there is first a suggestion of an ideomotor effect (e.g. "Your arm is getting stiff"), then a suggestion of an inhibition of control (e.g. "You cannot bend it"); and finally a test of the inhibition (e.g. "Go ahead, try to bend it"). Accordingly, as a rule, challenge suggestions are more difficult than direct suggestions; for different reasons, cognitive suggestions also tend to be more difficult than direct suggestions. In order to take account of differences in item difficulty, the AMSH items were rescored by adjusting the criteria for passing -- thus making the direct suggestions somewhat harder, and the challenge and cognitive suggestions somewhat easier (for details, see the normative study posted online).

The results of this alternative scoring are shown in Table 3. The patients again scored slightly lower than the students, but the overall difference was not statistically significant: t(114) = 1.19, n.s.). Interestingly, however, the difference was statistically significant for the six direct suggestions: t(114) = 3.53); corresponding differences for the challenge and cognitive suggestions were not significant (both ts < 1).

<<<<<Table 3 About Here>>>>>

Within the patient group, there was a tendency for subjects in the RH group to score somewhat higher on the direct suggestions than those in the LH group (t(14) = 1.78, p < .10); the differences in total score, and on the challenge and cognitive suggestions, did not approach statistical significance (all t < 1).

 

Discussion

The right-hemisphere hypothesis of hypnosis was based on early research that seemed to reveal dramatic differences in the capabilities of the two cerebral hemispheres. We now have a more nuanced view of the functional differences between the hemispheres (e.g., Efron, 1990), and we also appreciate the role typically performed by the left hemisphere in interpreting verbal suggestions from the hypnotist to the subject (Jasiukaitis, Nouriani, & Spiegel, 1996). Even so, there are reasons for entertaining the hypothesis that the two hemispheres play different roles in hypnosis. For example, in a variant on Sperry's (1968) view of double consciousness, Gazzaniga (1985, 1988) has proposed that consciousness is closely tied to the linguistic abilities normally associated functions with the left hemisphere. Accordingly, it might be that the right hemisphere unconsciously generates responses to hypnotic suggestions, which are then represented consciously, and interpreted, by the left hemisphere.

Regardless of the source of the right-hemisphere hypothesis, the present studies offer little or no evidence for it. For example, right-hemisphere damage due to stroke did not impair hypnotic responding. And while the performance of Patient GK indicated that the right hemisphere can support some degree of hypnotic responsiveness all by itself (provided that it possesses the requisite linguistic capacities), there was no evidence that the relative absence of a left hemisphere "disinhibited" right-hemisphere functions, so as to make him more hypnotizable than normal. If anything, right-hemisphere damage in the patients with unilateral stroke may have increased hypnotic responsiveness.

Although our sample was admittedly small, our failure to find significant differences between patients with right- and left-hemisphere lesions was not an artifact of low power. Given their scores on the MMSE, though, it is possible that the patients' strokes may not have been severe enough to significantly alter their hypnotizability.

As attractive as the right-hemisphere hypothesis may have been, it is possible that hemispheric specialization is not the right framework for neurological studies of hypnosis. In fact, it has been argued that a dorsal-ventral dichotomy is a better framework than the right-left dichotomy for organizing neuropsychological research -- with the dorsal system driven by expectations and processing action, and a ventral system driven by classification and processing perception (Borst, Thompson, & Kosslyn, 2011). Alternatively, an anterior-posterior dichotomy might better characterize the alterations in executive functioning that seem to underlie the "dissociative" phenomena of hypnosis -- involuntary movements, analgesia, amnesia, and the like. Perhaps the prefrontal cortex may play a special role in hypnosis (e.g., Farvolden & Woody, 2004; Halligan, Athwal, Oakley, & Frackowiak, 2000; Oakley & Halligan, 2009; Woody & McConkey, 2003). Lesions restricted to frontal cortex were notably absent in our sample of stroke patients.

Brain-imaging, whether by fMRI or other techniques, appears to be the favored means of approaching the question of the neural correlates of hypnosis (Oakley, 2008). At the same time, opportunities to study hypnosis in brain-injured patients should not be neglected. Historically, cognitive neuroscience has advanced by integrating neuroimaging studies of intact subjects with neuropsychological studies of brain-injured patients. The present small study shows that it is possible for brain-injured individuals to experience hypnosis, and that their responses to hypnosis can be assessed without unduly compromising the standardized procedures that, historically, put hypnosis research on a firm empirical base. Future studies of stroke patients employing larger samples to explore the dorsal/ventral and anterior/posterior dichotomies, and their potential interactions with lesion laterality, may help identify the neural substrates of hypnosis.

 

Author Notes

This research was supported by Grants #MH-35856, HD-055677, and RR-20146 from the National Institutes of Health.

 

References

Bakan, P. (1969). Hypnotizability, laterality of eye movements and functional brain asymmetry. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 28, 927-932.

Bakan, P. (1970). Handedness and hypnotizability. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 18, 99-104.

Barabasz, A. F., & Barabasz, M. (2008). Hypnosis and the brain. In M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), Oxford handbook of hypnosis (pp. 337-364). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Borst, G., Thompson, W. L., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2011). Understanding the dorsal and ventral systems of the human cerebral cortex: Beyond dichotomies. American Psychologist, 66(7), 624-632.

Cranney, J., & McConkey, K. M. (1980). Seating preference, hypnotizability, and imagery ability. Perceptual and motor skills, 50, 1159-1162.

Crawford, H. J. (1981). Hypnotic susceptibility as related to gestalt closure tasks. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 40(2), 376-383.

Crawford, H. J. (1986). Imagery processing during hypnosis: Relationships to hypnotizability and cognitive strategies. In M. Wolpin, J. E. Shorr & L. Krueger (Eds.), Imagery: Vol. 4. Recent practice and theory (pp. 13-32). New York: Plenum.

Crawford, H. J., & Gruzelier, J. H. (1992). A midstream view of the neuropsychophysiology of hypnosis: Recent research and future directions. In E. Fromm & M. R. Nash (Eds.), Contemporary hypnosis research (pp. 227-266). New York: Guilford.

Crawford, H. J., Gur, R. C., Skolnick, B., Gur, R. E., & Benson, D. M. (1993). Effects of hypnosis on regional cerebral blood flow during ischemic pain with and without suggested hypnotic analgesia. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 15, 181-195.

Edmonston, W. E., & Moskovitz, H. C. (1990). Hypnosis and lateralized brain function. International Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 38(1), 70-84.

Efron, R. (1990). The decline and fall of hemispheric specialization. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

Farvolden, P., & Woody, E. Z. (2004). Hypnosis, memory, and frontal executive functioning. International Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 52, 3-26.

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). "Mini-mental state": A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198.

Frumkin, L. R., Ripley, H. S., & Cox, G. B. (1978). Changes in cerebral hemispheric lateralization with hypnosis. Biological Psychiatry, 13, 741-750.

Gazzaniga, M. S. (1985). The social brain: Discovering the networks of the mind. New York: Basic Books.

Gazzaniga, M. S. (1988). Brain modularity: Toward a philosophy of conscious experience. In A. J. Marcel & E. Bisiach (Eds.), Consciousness in contemporary science (pp. 218-238). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Graffin, N. F., Ray, W. J., & Lundy, R. (1995). EEG concomitants of hypnosis and hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 123-131.

Graham, K. R., & Pernicano, K. (1979). Laterality, hypnosis and the autokinetic effect. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 22, 79-83.

Gruzelier, J., Brow, T., Perry, A., Rhonder, J., & Thomas, M. (1984). Hypnotic susceptibility: A lateral predisposition and altered cerebral asymmetry under hypnosis. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 2, 131-139.

Gruzelier, J., & Warren, K. (1993). Neuropsychological evidence of reductions on left frontal tests with hypnosis. Psychological Medicine, 23, 93-101.

Gruzelier, J. H., & Brow, T. D. (1985). Psychophysiological evidence for a state theory of hypnosis and susceptibility. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 29, 287-302.

Gur, R. C., & Gur, R. E. (1974). Handedness, sex, and eyedness as moderating variables in the relation between hypnotic susceptibility and functional brain asymmetry. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 28, 927-932.

Halligan, P. W., Athwal, B. S., Oakley, D. A., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (2000). Imaging hypnotic paralysis: Implications for conversion hysteria. Lancet, 355(March 18), 986-987.

Healey, F., Persinger, M. A., & Koren, S. A. (1996). Enhanced Hypnotic Suggestibility Following Application of Burst-Firing Magnetic Fields Over the Right Temporoparietal Lobes: A Replication. International Journal of Neuroscience, 87(3-4), 201-207.

Hellige, J. B. (1993). Hemispheric asymmetry: What's right and what's left. Cambridge, Ma.: Harvard University Pres.

Jasiukaitis, P., Nouriani, B., Hugdahl, K., & Spiegel, D. (1997). Relateralizing hypnosis; or Have we been barking up the wrong hemisphere? International Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 45(2), 158-177.

Jasiukaitis, P., Nouriani, B., & Spiegel, D. (1996). Left hemisphere superiority for event-related potential effects of hypnotic obstruction. Neuropsychologia, 34, 661-668.

Kihlstrom, J. F. (2008). The domain of hypnosis, revisited. In M. Nash & A. Barnier (Eds.), Oxford handbook of hypnosis (pp. 21-52). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Kihlstrom, J. F. (2011a). Arizona Motor Scale of Hypnotizability.  Link to normative study: http://socrates.berkeley.edu/~kihlstrm/AMSHS.htm.

Kihlstrom, J. F. (2011b). Neuro-hypnotism: Hypnosis and Neuroscience. Cortex, in press.

Kihlstrom, J. F., & Shor, R. E. (1978). Recall and recognition during posthypnotic amnesia. International Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 26(4), 330-349.

Laidlaw, T. M. (1993). Hypnosis and attention deficits after closed head injury. International Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 41, 97-111.

MacLeod-Morgan, C., & Lack, L. (1982). Hemispheric specificity: A physiological concomitant of hypnotizability. Psychophysiology, 19, 687-690.

Maquet, P., Faymonvi., M. E., DeGueldr., C., DelFiore, G., Franck, G., Luxen, A., et al. (1999). Functional neuroanatomy of hypnotic state. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 327-333.

Monteiro, K. P., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1987). The path from classroom seating to hypnotizability: A dead end. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 35, 82-86.

Morgan, A. H., Macdonald, H., & Hilgard, E. R. (1974). EEG alpha: Lateral asymmetry related to task and hypnotizability. Psychophysiology, 11, 275-282.

Morgan, A. H., Macdonald, P. J., & Macdonald, H. (1971). Differences in bilateral alpha activity as a function of experimental tasks, with a note on lateral eye movements and hypnotizability. Neuropsychologia, 9, 459-469.

Oakley, D. A. (2008). Hypnosis, trance and suggestion: Evidence from neuroimaging. In M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), Oxford handbook of hypnosis: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 366-392). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Oakley, D. A., & Halligan, P. W. (2009). Hypnotic suggestion ane cognitive neuroscience. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 264-270.

Otto-Salaj, L. L., Nadon, R., Hoyt, I. P., Register, P. A., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1992). Laterality of hypnotic response. International Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 40, 12-20.

Polster, M. R., & Rapcsak, S. Z. (1994). Hierarchical stimuli and hemispheric specialization: Two case studies. Cortex, 30, 487-497.

Query, W. T., Carlson, K., & Dreyer, S. (1983). Neuropsychological test performance and hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39, 804-806.

Rainville, P., Hofbauer, R. K., Paus, T., Bushnell, M. C., & Price, D. D. (1999). Cerebral mechanisms of hypnotic induction and suggestion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 110-125.

Rapcsak, S. Z., Beeson, P. M., & Rubens, A. B. (1991). Writing with the right hemisphere. Brain & Language, 41(4), 510-530.

Rapcsak, S. Z., Ochipa, C., Beeson, P. M., & Rubens, A. B. (1993). Praxis and the right hemisphere. Brain & Cognition, 23(2), 181-202.

Register, P. A., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1986). Finding the hypnotic virtuoso. International Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 34(2), 84-97.

Sackeim, H. A. (1982). Lateral asymmetry in bodily response to hypnotic suggestions. Biological Psychiatry, 17, 437-447.

Sackeim, H. A., Paulhus, D., & Weiman, A. L. (1979). Classroom seating and hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88(1), 81-84.

Shor, R. E., & Orne, E. C. (1962). Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form A. Palo Alto, Ca.:: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Shor, R. E., & Orne, E. C. (1963). Norms on the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form A. International Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 11, 39-47.

Spellacy, F., & Wilkinson, R. (1987). Dichotic listening and hypnotizability: Variability in ear preference. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 64, 1279-1284.

Sperry, R. W. (1968). Hemisphere deconnection and unity in conscious awareness. American Psychologist, 23, 723-733.

Stam, H. J., Spanos, N. P., Radtke, H. L., & Jones, B. (1981). Further investigation of the relationship between hypnotic susceptibility and classroom seating. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 52, 831-836.

Tiller, S. G., & Persinger, M. A. (1994). Enhanced hypnotizability by cerebrally applied magnetic fields depends upon the order of hemispheric presentation: An anisotropic effect. International Journal of Neuroscience, 79, 157-163.

Wallace, B., & Persanyi, M. W. (1989). Hypnotic susceptibility and familial handedness. Journal of General Psychology, 116, 345-350.

Weitzenhoffer, A. M., & Hilgard, E. R. (1959). Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale, Forms A and B. Palo Alto, Ca.: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Weitzenhoffer, A. M., & Hilgard, E. R. (1962). Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale, Form C. Palo Alto, Ca.: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Woody, E. Z., & McConkey, K. M. (2003). What We Don't Know About the Brain and Hypnosis, But Need to: A View from the Buckhorn Inn. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 51(3), 309-337.

 

 

Table 1

Radiological Findings in Stroke Patients

N

Distribution of Lesions

7 of 9 Patients with Left-Hemisphere Lesions

3

Frontal-Temporal-Parietal (Cortical and Subcortical)

2

Temporal-Parietal

1

Occipital-Temporal

1

Posterior Thalamus

4 of 7 Patients with Right-Hemisphere Lesions

2

Frontal-Temporal-Parietal (Cortical and Subcortical)

1

Temporal-Parietal

1

Occipital

 

 

Table 2

Hypnotizability Scale Performance:

Standard Scoring

Variable

Group

M

SD

Comparing Patients and Students

Total Score

Patients

Students

8.56

8.25

3.63

2.74

6 Direct Suggestions*

Patients

Students

4.47

4.06

1.55

1.06

6 Challenge Suggestions

Patients

Students

2.04

2.19

1.78

1.60

3 Cognitive Suggestions

Patients

Students

1.16

1.06

1.00

0.93

Comparing Patients with Right- and Left-Hemisphere Damage

Total Score

Right

Left

8.86

7.78

2.41

3.03

6 Direct Suggestions*

Right

Left

4.57

3.67

0.53

1.22

6 Challenge Suggestions

Right

Left

2.14

2.22

1.68

1.64

3 Cognitive Suggestions

Right

Left

1.14

1.00

0.90

1.00

*Excludes Eye Closure suggestion administered during induction procedure.

 

 

Table 3

Hypnotizability Scale Performance:

Alternative Scoring

Variable

Lesion

M

SD

Comparing Patients and Students

Total Score

Patients

Students

7.59

6.31

4.13

3.03

6 Direct Suggestions*

Patients

Students

2.96

1.25

1.91

0.68

6 Challenge Suggestions

Patients

Students

2.75

2.88

1.91

1.63

3 Cognitive Suggestions

Patients

Students

1.32

1.50

1.00

0.96

Comparing Patients with Right- and Left-Hemisphere Damage

Total Score

Right

Left

7.14

5.67

2.34

3.46

6 Direct Suggestions*

Right

Left

1.57

1.00

0.53

0.71

6 Challenge Suggestions

Right

Left

3.29

2.56

1.50

1.74

3 Cognitive Suggestions

Right

Left

1.57

1.44

0.98

1.01

*Excludes Eye Closure suggestion administered during induction procedure.